Gananath Obeyesekere’s 1975 Article on Murder by Sorcery – By Michael Roberts

Gananath Obeyesekere’s 1975 Article on Murder by Sorcery – By Michael Roberts

Gananath Obeyesekere: “Sorcery and Premeditated Murder: The Canalization of Aggression”[1]

elanka

Source:thuppahis.com

Michael RobertsIn this paper I want to deal with a series of interrelated problems beginning with the following specific questions and propositions. First: how far can we make inferences about the human psyche and social structure from official statistics computed by government agencies, in this case statistics on homicide and crimes of violence? Criminology as a discipline is especially concerned with this problem, and criminological studies in Sri Lanka have made social structural, cultural and psychological inferences from the statistical data.[2]  At the outset, let me emphasize that I am not concerned with the conventional debate about the accuracy of governmental statistics. Criminologist who have dealt with this issue are agreed that Sri Lanka’s official statistics on homicide and violent crimes are reasonably accurate, and on the face of it there is perfect justification for using these data for social analysis.

Second: homicide and violent crimes are the expression of the hostile impulses and actions of people, and as such they pertain to the larger problems of how aggression is handled, controlled, and canalized in any human society. We cannot fully understand the problem of crimes of violence unless we analyze the more fundamental problem of the expression of aggression in society. Social scientists who have studied crime in Western societies will at once object, and state with some justification that violent crimes are a special type of hostile activity, radically different from, let us say, football or boxing, and one which merits analysis independently of other forms of aggression. However, the position changes quite remarkably when we examine traditional and contemporary non-Western societies. It can be easily demonstrated that in many such societies there are modes of killing or hurting people other than by use of lethal weapons or poison — by those magical means known as sorcery and witchcraft. In this paper we are concerned with sorcery only which we define it as a “technique of killing or harming someone, deliberately and intentionally, generally with homeopathic or contagious magic, accompanied by spells, charms, or incantations.” Thus sorcery can be compared with homicide and crimes of violence, if not in its actual effect, at least in its motive and intended effect.

Third: sorcery is a deliberate and intentional act because the person who consults a sorcerer does so after considerable planning and premeditation. In this sense sorcery is calculated and one might say it is a “rational crime,” using rational in Max Weber’s sense of “practical rationality,” that is, a careful consideration of means for achieving a given end. The end in this case being murder or personal harm to a “victim.” The rational nature of sorcery is of crucial importance for the present paper, since it stands in clear contrast to the dominant pattern of the homicide and “grave crime” rate computed officially, and to the questionnaire data of criminologists.

Criminologists who have worked in Sri Lanka unequivocally postulate that spontaneous and unpremeditated crime totally outweighs premeditated homicide and violence. Wood shows clearly that only 20 per cent of all cases of homicide are premeditated, 51 per cent of the cases are spontaneous but with previous enmity or dispute, and 29 per cent are spontaneous without previous enmity and dispute.[3] Bloch substantially agrees with him.[4] A spontaneous homicidal act is one in which the time gap between stimulus and response is narrow, giving the individual little time to weigh rationally the implications of his action. In Sri Lanka, spontaneous homicide and violence often erupt under trivial provocation and might result in brutal and instantaneous response[5]; and this is probably true for many parts of the world.

In this paper I will show that the low frequency of premeditation in homicide and violence in the official record neither implies that the motive for premeditated crime is infrequent nor that its expression is infrequent. It only implies that the motives for such acts are canalized through what is sometimes called “functional alternatives.” In Sri Lanka, as in many parts of the non-Western world, that alternative lies in the practice of sorcery. But if this is indeed the case, then official statistics show only one side of the picture. Crime in Sri Lanka is presented as spontaneous, not as rational and calculated, as it sometimes can be according to the evidence we shall present later. Furthermore, insofar as such data are one-sided and biased, the inferences made from them regarding psychological motivations and social structure must also perforce be biased and one-sided, such as Bloch’s interpretation of spontaneous crime as a function of low frustration tolerance or a loose social structure.[6]

The problem investigated

The major problem of this paper leads us to consider the possibility that the same function might be served by substantively different structures. Thus the same set of motives may be canalized and expressed through different customs or cultural forms, and the same custom may be a vehicle for the expression of a different set of motives. These questions have serious implications for any kind of correlational analysis, in that totally misleading results may emerge if there is no control over the functional alternative (if and when it exists) of any custom or trait that is used as a variable. The problem is confounded further if any particular cultural form or custom or set of practices is not only a functional alternative to another but also its structural alternative, i.e., if the two customs are isomorphic. This implies that the data gathered with respect to one custom or practice would be quite misleading if the data relevant to the alternative are ignored, since both types of data deal with substantively the same phenomenon. Undoubtedly it would be unusual to find a single custom or practice serving as both a structural and functional alternative to another, i.e., a nearly complete isomorph.  However, I submit that sorcery and premeditated murder and calculated violent crime are structurally isomorphic and functionally similar in certain important dimensions or parameters.

Let me specify these dimensions.  First, there is the intention of the actor. In premeditated homicide and violence as well as in sorcery, the intention of the actor is the same, which is to kill or cause personal harm to the victim. In Sri Lanka, people can explicitly verbalize their hostile intentions, such that when they go to some of the more powerful sorcerers, the homicidal intention is clear. They want their enemies killed.  Second, there is the rational nature of the act. In both sorcery and premeditated murder there is a real time gap between stimulus and response. During the interim period the offender makes rational decisions regarding the kind of sorcery he should practice, the sorcerer he should consult, and the time, effort, expense, and risk involved. The sorcerer is, in effect, the equivalent of a hired killer. Third, there is the traditional legal definition. Traditionally, both sorcery and murder were viewed as crimes and carried the identical punishment and this provides further evidence that in pre-British times these two practices were seen as similar. Ralph Pieris noted that in the time of the Kandyan kings (before 1815), “several persons were executed and their lands given to the injured party” as penalty for practicing sorcery.[7] Fourth, when we consider the techniques employed we are compelled to recognize the first noticeable substantive difference. In both the techniques are viewed as pragmatic and instrumental, but in one “poison, arson, or the knife” are employed, while the other uses magical practices which vary from one type of sorcery to another. In evaluating the difference between the two techniques employed, one must not make the mistake of seeing magical practices in sorcery from the Western intellectualist viewpoint as entirely consisting of symbolic and expressive actions. Sinhala persons are aware of the substantive differences between physical weapons and magical weapons, but the employment of both are viewed as pragmatic and instrumental techniques completely different from the expressive forms of worship in, let us say, a Buddhist monastery. Moreover, they are aware that sorcery is not all of a piece, and that some forms of sorcery are more powerful than others. I am convinced that many Sinhalas would view the sorcery practiced by some of the well-known vas-kavi sorcerers (practitioners of “poison verses” discussed later) as effective and deadly as hiring a paid assassin and sometimes even more so for the believer. Also, the risks involved in premeditated murder or violence against the person must be weighed against the relative safety of sorcery because in modern Sri Lankan law sorcery is no longer a crime; and is practiced in secret and also less messy than actual acts of personal violence. The one truly significant difference between sorcery and the premeditated crime is its relation to the consequence, because we know and can demonstrate that in fact people rarely die as a consequence of sorcery. But people believe that it is effective and hence they would rather practice sorcery than commit a direct act of physical violence. In other words, the motive for premeditated murder can be channeled away from physical violence into sorcery. If so, the present data on crime statistics with their high ratio of spontaneous crimes to premeditated ones are highly biased and might be corrected by taking into account the factor of sorcery.

In order to do this, one has to have statistical data on the incidence of the practice of sorcery. I have emphasized the term practice, in order to distinguish it from imputations or accusations of sorcery. In some societies such as the Navaho, sorcery accusations or imputations are universal, but there is no real evidence regarding its actual practice.[8] In other societies, such as Sri Lanka, India, and the Philippines, people not only accuse others of practicing sorcery, but actually do practice it. Anthropologists have generally analyzed the pattern of sorcery accusations or imputations but not their practice, largely because their practice is generally veiled under a shroud of secrecy and their incidence is difficult to determine. In this paper we shall attempt to estimate the incidence of sorcery practice.

We encountered several initial difficulties. The most deadly form of sorcery practice in Sri Lanka is known as vas-kavi (vas meaning “ritual danger,” “poison,” or “malignant action” and kavi meaning “songs”). Vas-kavi are songs constructed on the basis of highly elaborate astrological and other esoteric calculations; each song is designed for the specific needs of the client. These songs are then sung a prescribed number of times (generally 108 times or multiples thereof) before the image of some dominant deity like the god Skanda (Kataragama). Ironically, many vas-kavi specialists are Buddhist monks, some of them having great reputations as sorcerers.[9] For example, some believe that the moment an especially powerful vas-kavi specialist performs the prescribed ritual and steps out of the shrine the victim falls dead! Vas-kavi is very popular among politicians as a technique of getting rid of political enemies.[10] The fees charged by the better known vas-kavi sorcerers are also high, and only the affluent can generally afford their services.[11] None of the monks who practiced vas-kavi were willing to talk about their clientele, and we could not get any useful information on the incidence of sorcery practice from this source.

Next to vas-kavi are the cruder forms of sorcery known as hūniyan. These are consistent with practices recorded in the cross-cultural literature and abound with homeopathic and contagious magic, e.g., pricking thorns into the wax image of the victim, burying charmed objects in the garden of the victim, or using the hair and other bodily exuviae of the victim. Hūniyan is generally performed by ritual specialists known as a kaṭtadirāla, whose major ritual role, however, is the propitiation of demons that have afflicted individuals. Most of the kaṭtadirālas we knew were performers of

benevolent curative magic, but occasionally some of them would perform hūniyan in order to please an influential client or earn a large fee. The data we obtained from this source, although interesting, were too few and hence inadequate for our purposes.  Thus the only firm conclusion we could draw from our knowledge of vas-kavi and hūniyan specialists was that any person with enough money to pay the sorcerer could, in Sri Lanka, readily use vas-kavi or hūniyan with the intention of causing death or serious harm to an individual or group of individuals. However, it remains a conclusion that cannot be supported by numerical data.

A third type of sorcery is associated with shrines presided over by a powerful deity or deities. The client consults the ritual specialist of the shrine who enlists the aid of the deity in order to cause death or harm to the victim. These ritual centers were excellently suited for our purposes for the following reasons:

  1. a) These shrines were public places which did not conduct their rituals in secret.
  2. b) The fees charged by the specialists varied from one shrine to another and in relation to the nature of the client’s problem, but in general they were within the means of an average villager.
  3. c) Large numbers of clients visit these shrines, so that it was possible to get a sample large enough for our purpose.
  4. d) Many of these shrines were so famous nationally or regionally as centers for the practice of sorcery that they drew clients from various parts of the country or a large region. Therefore, they lacked the intimate character of village monasteries or shrines. The clients who visited a shrine did not generally know one another and they could therefore maintain a degree of anonymity. Because the maintenance of anonymity was very important for them, clients visiting these public sorcery shrines ensured anonymity through geographic distance. The incidence of the use of distance is presented in Table I.

Sorcery must be practiced in secret; yet, paradoxically, sorcery shrines are public places. Clients can best ensure that they will not meet people known to them at the sorcery shrine by going to a distant one. Hence people who live in the vicinity of a shrine rarely visit it.  In Table 1 only the cases in the first column risk being found out; the rest (8o per cent of the cases) are fairly certain of maintaining their anonymity. Those distances are especially significant when we realize that Sri Lanka is a small country of about 24,000 square miles. The Muslim shrine has the largest proportion of persons within a five mile radius. This shrine is located in an almost exclusively Muslim town, hence Buddhists and Hindus from nearby villages could come there without much fear of recognition.

Research method

My original intention was to work with vas-kavi sorcerers, but for reasons mentioned earlier we had to work at the sorcery shrines instead. We picked three shrines, each representing one of the important religions of the nation–Buddhist, Hindu, and Muslim.

TABLE 1

Number of Clients Living Within Specified Distances from Sorcery Shrines

5 Mile Radius5-10 Miles10-50 Miles50 Miles &Over
No.%No.%No.%No.%No.%
Hindu Shrine83.783.712759.07233.4 215100
Muslim Shrine5412.36715.228063.9378.4438100
Buddhist Shrine107.897.03325.77659.3128100
Total729.28410.844056.318523.7781100

 

elanka

The Buddhist shrine is located at Sinigama in the southwest coast of Sri Lanka, about fifty miles from Colombo. The presiding deity of the shrine is Devol Deviyo, viewed by the Buddhists of the area as a major god in their pantheon. The Hindu shrine is located at Munnesvaram in the northwest coast of Sri Lanka, 55 miles from Colombo. Munnesvaram is one of the most famous of Hindu monasteries in Sri Lanka, sacred to Lord Siva. However, the practice of sorcery, which often involves the slaughter of a goat or chicken, takes place in the Kālī shrine located outside the main monastery, and the ritual specialists are non-Brahmin. The scale of the sacrifice was consonant with the intention of the client — a chicken indicated physical harm to the victim (accident, illness), whereas a goat indicated death. Persons who were willing to take no chances sacrificed several goats to the deity. (These animals were subsequently sold to the local restaurants by the priests). The Muslim shrine is part of a mosque at Kahatapitiya, six miles from the University of Sri Lanka campus, on the main Peradeniya-Gampola road. The rituals are performed in a room where the body of a local Muslin saint is buried.

The shrines are not exclusive to the particular religions, but are open to all. In fact most of the clients in both the Muslim and Hindu shrines were Buddhist, demographically the largest segment of the country’s population. This again indicates that sorcery is viewed as an instrumental rather than expressive type of activity by the clients who patronize these shrines.

The research was designed in two stages. The initial stage was the collection of data from the three shrines: these data would be simple, lacking in the depth characteristic of case studies of individuals. Our intention in the second stage was to refine some of the conclusions that emerged from the first study, subject these hypotheses to a more rigorous test, and also obtain selected case studies in depth. For this purpose we picked a sorcery shrine in the Moravak Korale in the southern Province. This shrine was atop a huge mountain in a heavily forested region, and we felt that anyone who could undertake this backbreaking hike must be singularly steadfast in his intentions. However, this study was abandoned as the area was overrun by insurgents in the April 1971 insurrection.

The research design was extremely simple. Research assistants were asked to collect information for a period of 6o days in each shrine. Two research assistants were placed in Munnesvaram because we felt this was the most important shrine whereas one research assistant was placed in each of the other shrines. Since daily rituals were performed in both Munnesvaram and the Kahatapitiya mosque, we had the necessary information collected, with minor breaks, within a period of slightly over two months. The Buddhist shrine had a different routine. In general the public belief was that Wednesdays and Saturdays were the special days (known as kemmura) and reserved for worship of the gods. However, the priest said that this applies for benevolent magic and propitiatory ritual only. His view was that sorcery rituals could be practiced any day. The upshot of this confusion was that most clients visited the shrine on Wednesdays and Saturdays, although a few came on other days, while on some days there were no clients. Thus, at the Buddhist shrine 6o days of field work extended to a four-month period.

Owing to the sensitive nature of the subject matter, the research assistants were instructed to collect extremely simple information on the following topics.

  1. Purpose of visit
  2. Who was responsible for the harm done to the client (for provoking him to practice sorcery)?
  3. Was any action taken by the client before coming to the shrine?
  4. Number of persons in the clients group, their gender and approximate ages.
  5. Where ever possible, information on ethnic background, religion and civil status of the client.

In fact our research assistant did not have to interview the clients directly because most of the above information in both Munnesvaram and Sinigama were supplied by the priests themselves who routinely elicit this information with, however, a little bit of encouragement on our part. Thus in these two places the interviewing was done for us by the priests. In fact some priests reframed their conventional questions so as to get unequivocal answers for our benefit. Thus the basic information was collected in as normal a manner as possible without upsetting the social situation at the shrine premises. I had instructed our assistants to gather further sociological information from the clients after they had finished their rituals (pūja) in the shrines. This was extremely difficult in Munnesvaram owing to the large numbers that came here, but considerable information was nevertheless obtained. At Sinigama the numbers were smaller, the priest was most co-operative and well-known to me, and we were able to get extra sociological information which added depth to our study and permitted the testing of many hypotheses not anticipated at the time the research was originally formulated. The situation at the mosque was slightly different. Contrary to our expectations, the mosque had an extremely large number of clients. Furthermore, the priest did not ask the basic questions and our research assistant, with the permission of the mosque authorities was permitted to take on an official role and this meant that he had to gather the information himself. Consequently it was virtually impossible to get detailed information, except very basic material from the Muslim shrine. To sum up; the most detailed sociological information came from Sinigama, then Munnesvaram and lastly from the Kahatapitiya mosque. However, in all three places we had sufficient information on the basic questions mentioned above.

Test of the major hypothesis

While the crime statistics reveal that 72 per cent of the homicides and crimes of violence are spontaneous and 20 per cent premeditated, we hypothesize that a radically different pattern will be revealed when the data on sorcery are taken into account (see Table 2).

Table 2 shows a total of 803 cases of interviewed clients who came to the three shrines during the 60 days of field work in each shrine with an estimated yearly total 7,441.  The significance of these figures comes out dramatically when we compare the four categories of violent crimes for Sri Lanka for the year 1957, to use a convenient date.[12] Wood shows that 6,477 violent crimes (homicide, attempted homicide, grievous hurt, hurt by knife) occurred in that year; if we add arson (566) and riot (89) we have a total of 7,023. By contrast the yearly figure for sorcery for just three shrines is more than the total figure for all violent crime in the year 1957!  If we view sorcery practice as the equivalent of premeditated violence, then this category of behavior is more common than spontaneous violence. Such a view of human behavior seems to us to be perfectly justified. In every human society people are taught to control two of our most imperious drives, namely, sex and aggression. It is true that the degree of control and the nature of the inhibitory mechanisms vary from one society to another. Yet it seems to us that in every society there are those who, when confronted with provocative stimuli, act impulsively and aggressively, while others have learned to control or repress these impulses and bring them under the control of rational judgment. It can be presumed that in to the latter outnumber the former. Viewed in this light, the problem of premeditation in the perpetration of crime must be placed in a larger perspective than criminologists have hitherto done.

TABLE 2

Total Number of Clients Visiting Three Sorcery Shrines for a 60 Day Period, and an Estimated Yearly Total

No of shrines & duration of workTotal no. of clients visiting shrinesNo. of Clients InterviewedNo. of Interviewed Clients Who Came for SorceryPercentageof Clients of sorceryNo. of  Clients Who Came for other purposesEstimated yearly total of  sorcery
Hindu shrine

(2 month period)

 

915

32824876804,150
Muslim shrine

(2 month period)

600

(approximately)

 

539

 

442

 

82

 

97

 

2,952

Buddhist shrine

(4 month period)

 

128

 

128

 

113

 

88

 

15

 

339

Total1,643995803811927,441

The problem of viewing crime as premeditated versus spontaneous clouds the larger issue. When people are confronted with stimuli that may provoke them to a homicidal or violent response, the following reactions probably take place. The largest numbers of people repress, suppress or control the desire or drive for violence and having done so, bring the whole problem under the control of rational thinking and the moral norms that govern their consciences.  However, a very small proportion of people confronted with identical stimuli act in a somewhat different, yet “rational” manner. They control the impulse to act out immediately with violence, yet they might plan violence or murder in a premeditated manner that will bring about the desired end with the maximum efficiency and expediency. To sum up, many people when confronted with provocative stimuli are tempted to commit murder or violence against another; yet most people repress or suppress these wishes and bring them under the control of reason; while a small number commit what is known as premeditated murder, and a larger group practice spontaneous murder and violence.

These considerations bring us to another feature of the sorcery data that are presented in Table 2, that is, the yearly total for three shrines is a mere fraction of all the sorcery actually practiced in the country. This is obvious, because we have ignored the hundreds of specialists who practice the most deadly kind of sorcery, vas-kavi; the thousands of kaṭtaḍirāla (priests of the demon cults) who practice hūniyan type sorcery at least occasionally; and furthermore probably over a dozen national and regional shrines similar to the ones we studied. It should also be noted that while the priests of the village dēvāles (shrines for gods) generally deal with benevolent magic, they also on occasion deal with sorcery. Insofar as there are thousands of such dēvāles, this will also add to the picture of the incidence of sorcery we have presented. Thus we are confronted with a curious problem. If sorcery on the motivational level is in some sense an equivalent of premeditated crime, then this category totally outweighs the total number of cases of spontaneous crime, and the statistical relationship between premeditated and spontaneous crime noted by criminologists is reversed. But this again is implausible for, according to criminologists (and here we agree with them), the incidence of spontaneous violence in any country is larger than that of premeditated homicide and violence. We advance the following proposition to account for this disproportion: if any society provides culturally constituted mechanisms for the canalization of the aggressive impulses of individuals, then those individuals who would normally tend to repress or suppress these impulses and bring them under rational control would now be tempted to give them expression. 

Several objections can be leveled against the preceding arguments. Some likely theoretical objections will be considered at the conclusion of this paper. Here we will consider an important substantive and factual objection which may be raised. In the case of premeditated (or for that matter in spontaneous) crime, the target of the attack is a known identifiable enemy. Is it the same for the sorcery cases collected from the three shrines? It is possible to argue that only insofar as the enemy could be identified could the two types of data be considered truly isomorphic. Table 3 gives the relevant information from the three shrines. In the Buddhist and Muslim shrines information relating to this problem is available from all the respondents; in the Hindu shrine it is available for 90 per cent of the total sample. I think it makes no difference psychologically whether one clearly identifies the enemy or guesses his identity (both could be treated in a single category of “enemy known”). Thus in 66 per cent of the cases in all three shrines the enemy is “known” to the client; in 34 per cent of the cases the enemy is unknown.

How does this information affect the two hypotheses stated earlier?

  1. a) At worst the number of sorcery cases presented in Table 1 must be reduced by 34 per cent so that we have a yearly total of only 4911. This also means that the number of cases of sorcery is less than the yearly crime rate for 1957 which in any case was an arbitrary baseline.

TABLE 3

Identity of the Enemy Percentages of the Cases for which Information is Available

No.Per Cent Clearly IdentifiedPer Cent Identity         GuessedPer Cent Enemy                                      UnknownTotal
Munnesvaram223551629100
Kahatapitiya44249843100
Sinigama113402931100
Total778481834100

However our basic hypothesis is not affected, for there is nevertheless an extraordinarily large number of sorcery cases and a radically different pattern still emerges even when those data reduced by 34 per cent are considered. Insofar as there are many other sorcery shrines and types of sorcery, this form of “rational crime” would totally and completely out-shadow spontaneous crime.

  1. b) On the other hand, I would like to argue that the 34 per cent who cannot identify their enemy are relevant for the present study, for an important reason. I shall demonstrate later that a majority of these clients are persons not given to spontaneous emotional responses. Thus I believe that most of these clients simply do not proceed to find out who was responsible for causing them harm because such a quest might eventually lead them into a personal confrontation which they are not psychologically equipped to handle. Thus potentially they are capable of identifying their enemies and therefore there is no serious categorical distinction between them and the others in our sorcery sample.

Social status and sorcery practice 

According to Bloch, Wood, and Jayewardene in the aforementioned studies, disputes over land and sex are the main provocations underlying criminal homicide; disputes over other types of property come a close third. Our analyses of the disputes underlying sorcery (using the Sinigama data) show that the overwhelmingly large reason for practicing sorcery is dispute over property, generally theft or damage done to property. For example, in Sinigama nine cases were scored for land dispute and nine for sex dispute, while 86 were scored for property disputes. According to Wood, sex and land disputes constitute respectively 17 and 16 per cent of the immediate causes of homicide.[13]   One methodological reason for the disparity is obvious: theirs is a national sample, whereas ours deals with three shrines, and can give no accurate information on the nature of sorcery practice in Sri Lanka in general. However, a substantive reason for the difference, as far as the Sinigama data are concerned, is that the majority of people who visit the shrine are not village farmers, but rather those who practice non-traditional occupations. It seems that, in our sample at least, those who practice sorcery tend to come from the more urbanized, probably more educated, and more enterprising segments of village society. The aspect of our data given in Table 4 has very interesting and important implications.

TABLE 4

Occupations of Clients Visiting the Sinigama Shrine

1.Farmer22(17.0%)
2.Other traditional occupation 3(2.3 %)
3.Landed proprietor16(12.4 %)
4.Shopkeeper12(9.3%)
5.Other non-traditional occupations40(31.0 %)
6.No information36(27.9 %)
Total129(99.9 %)

Number 1 is the strictly traditional occupation of village farmers. Number 2 has two traditional occupations of Buddhist monk and exorcist (kaṭtadirāla). Number 3 needs some explanation. These are landowners who though living in the village have emancipated themselves from the traditional occupation of village farmers, and have cultivated cash crops like tea, rubber, and coconut. They are reasonably wealthy and educated and do not consider themselves to be cultivators or farmers. Number 4, consisting of shopkeepers, are obviously non-traditional. To get the full flavor of the non-traditional occupations in Number 5, it is well worth listing some of the occupations involved: mason, garage owner, automobile mechanic, proprietor of handloom shop, rice mill owner, bīdi manufacturer, seamstress, taxi driver, village committee member, estate laborer, teacher, printing press owner, hospital attendant, government contractor, car salesman, pawnbroker, port worker, university graduate, tailor, salesman, storekeeper, typist, railway ticket collector, contractor, typewriter mechanic, and government clerk.[14]

Thus one reason for the high incidence of disputes pertaining to property, other than land, is that the sorcery sample does not reflect the true occupational composition of the country, which is subsistence farming, but tends to be biased in the “urban” direction. Even the category of people who are classed as landed proprietors rarely come to Sinigama because of land disputes, for it can be assumed that these wealthy people owning substantial areas of land would not have the problem of farmers, which in general is one of land shortage. Rather, landed proprietors practice sorcery because an enemy has destroyed their cash crops or stolen cattle or some other kind of property.

The data on social mobility permitted us to inquire whether our assumption that sorcery practice showed a bias in the “urban” or “middle class” direction was correct. Thirteen clients stated that they were outsiders in the village or town in which they resided, and as such were harassed by the locals. This information was given voluntarily during the interview, and it is likely that more cases would have fallen into this category if we had had a specific question on the subject in our schedule. What is significant, however, is that all (except one for whom no information is available) who considered themselves as outsiders in the village come from the non-traditional occupations. Furthermore, the data indicated that these outsiders were confined to those groups — shopkeepers and other non-traditional occupations — that rep\resent the most “urban” of the groups in our sample. Significantly, no one in the landed proprietor group stated they were outsiders, because as the author has shown elsewhere, typical landed proprietors are village residents who have made good through several generations and represent a village aristocracy and intelligentsia.[15] That many of the clients are outsiders is also indirectly confirmed by the data presented in Table 5, which lists the clients’ enemies (victims) in terms of kin/non-kin distinction.  If 73 per cent of the enemies are non-kin, it is also likely that a fair proportion of the clients would also be outsiders to the village. Furthermore, the data clearly show that we are not dealing with traditional villages structured on a reasonably homogenous kinship base but rather with changed and highly acculturated communities.

TABLE 5

Identity of Victims of Sorcery in Terms of Kin/Nonkin Criteria

Name of ShrinePer Cent Kin                          Per Cent Non-kin
Hindu Shrine2080
Muslim Shrine2872
Buddhist Shrine3367
Total2773

 The fact that the sorcery sample is more “urban” than rural has significant implications. The term urban is somewhat misleading and I must explain what I mean by it. Clients in general come from villages or from the small townships adjacent to a group of villages. But occupationally they constitute a new class in the village social structure, in general analogous to “lower middle class” in Western stratification terminology. Although not a city population, they constitute a highly acculturated segment of village society. What is at issue here is not the fact of living in an urban area but rather that categories 3, 4 and 5 in Table 4 reflect in general a class of people who are gainfully employed, economically well off, hardworking, and perhaps thrifty — the good middle class of the village or small town. Such people are generally law-abiding citizens. Sri Lanka’s official crime statistics in both Wood and Bloch also prove that homicide and attempted homicide are typically a village (rural) problem and that the rate is lowest in the metropolitan Colombo region, at least for the periods 1960 and 1961It is true that the Colombo region has a high rate for violence other than homicide but this in our opinion reflects a tendency of the urban proletariat, rather than the middle class. Our data clearly show that the motives for homicide and attempted homicide exist in the urbanized sections of Sri Lanka’s population, but these motives are canalized and expressed through the mechanism of sorcery. Since this is the case, it should again be realized that danger exists in drawing psychological and societal inferences from the official census data as Bloch and Wood have done. Our data furthermore show that middle class or occupationally “urban” people in Ceylon tend not to commit spontaneous homicide or violence, but rather tend to commit sorcery, which is a form of rational crime.

Sorcery practice and personality type

The foregoing analyses of sorcery data further suggest the validity of the following conclusion: the kind of people who practice sorcery, whether urban or villager, have considerable inner control and are not given to spontaneous violence when confronted with provocative stimuli. This conclusion is borne out very clearly in the data for the three shrines. If our view is correct, the data we have should enable us to test a further hypothesis: the personalities of individuals practicing sorcery are such that when confronted with provocative stimuli they tend to control their impulses rather than resort to personal retaliation. This hypothesis is clearly confirmed by the data categorized in Table 6. In this table we have, for obvious reasons, eliminated cases where the enemy is not known to the client, or where the information was uncertain.

The data presented in Table 6 are very impressive. In only 10 per cent of the cases did the client confront his enemy personally, even though he could identify his enemy or believed he could. Moreover, in 6 per cent of the cases there was merely a face-to-face meeting in order to “persuade,” or to “explain.” In the Sinigama sample, where we have detailed case studies, these attempts at confronting the enemy were not always advantageous to the client, because in five instances the enemy in fact retaliated with physical or verbal abuse against the client. Only 4 per cent of the sample were truly aggressive: they not only confronted the enemy but also retaliated with verbal or physical abuse (mostly verbal, there being only two cases of physical retaliation in the whole sample). We feel that we are truly dealing with a type of individual who has the capacity to control his impulses and act with considerable restraint. This is further borne out in the Sinigama data. Here we have over a dozen cases where the clients have been pushed to the wall by their enemies through continual physical, verbal, and other forms of intimidation. Yet they have patiently borne the ignominy and have not immediately retaliated in kind; rather they have calculated their moves and postponed their retaliation through supernatural and magical means.

We are also impressed by another aspect of client behavior. A considerable number of people had taken some form of official action, such as informing the headman or police, or seeking court redress: 31 per cent at Munnesvaram, 22 per cent at Kahatapitiya and 33 per cent at Sinigama. This is even more impressive because practically everyone who expressed opinions on the subject spoke of the futility of reporting to the police. Respondents were unanimous in their view that police officers were corrupt and under the control of “undesirables” and politicians. In any case, villagers are extremely afraid of the police, both for rational and non-rational reasons. In general we believe that villagers were quite justified in disassociating themselves from the police, because it is unlikely that the police would give them a sympathetic ear. At least one thing is clear: the very fact that people define the police as corrupt, brutal, and unreliable prevent more of them from seeking police help. Yet the fact remains that in spite of these views 29 per cent sought police and other official help which suggests that we are dealing with individuals who seek rational solutions to their problems.

Sorcery and the quest for justice

In almost all of our cases the client practiced sorcery as a retaliation to an actual wrong done to him by someone else. An examination of the interview schedules confirms, without any doubt, that this is simply not the kind of rationalization phenomenon one encounters in murderers. It is well-known that those who commit criminal homicide tend to justify the rightness of their action.

TABLE 6

Confrontation of the Enemy by the Client

Personal

Confrontation

Physical or

Verbal Abuse

No ConfrontationTotal  Number

of Cases

 Munnesvaram11 (6.9%)2 (1.2%)145 (91.7%)158
 Kahatapitiya5 (1.9 %)13 (5. 1 %)234 (92.8%)252
 Sinigama14 (18.0 %)4 (5. 1%)60 (77.0%)78
Total30 (6%)19 (4 %)439 (90 %)488

Our sorcery clients too felt that they were the victims of injustice and that their retaliatory sorcery action was justified. However, these subjectively felt justifications for their actions, we feel, had an objective validity in most instances. For example: a spouse had been seduced by another; property had been stolen or wantonly damaged; or a person had been harassed or bullied. In the few cases where the client practiced sorcery because someone else had practiced sorcery on him, the objective reality of the latter incident might be false. But at least the client subjectively felt he had been the victim of an unjust sorcery attack that demanded just retaliation in kind.

Furthermore, there were no mechanisms for resolving these conflicts on the local level. The traditional authority structure in the changing villages and small towns of Sri Lanka have almost totally disappeared, so that there are no institutional mechanisms for resolving conflicts and redressing wrongs done to a person. Institutions like the caste panchayats of India do not exist here, and the kinship groups, which traditionally may have helped resolve some of these conflicts, are rift by internal tension, largely as a result of land and inheritance disputes.[16]  Up to 1963, Sri Lankan villages had a headman, generally recruited from the local aristocracy, and these headmen sometimes acted as arbitrators in village disputes. But the headman system was abolished in 1963 and the new headman system (grāmasēvaka, village server), where recruitment is on the basis of competitive examinations, was introduced. The loss of the older headman was the final blow to the traditional institutional structure. The new headmen were simply bureaucratic officers on the village level incapable of arbitrating or resolving local conflicts. The government, realizing the seriousness of the problem, has introduced an institution known as a “Conciliation Committee.” However, it is obvious that these official Conciliation Committees have failed, for none of our clients admitted going to these committees to resolve his or her problems. The old authority structure has broken down, the problems faced by people have multiplied, and there exists no satisfactory alternative structure of authority. The police are distrusted and the courts are too impersonal and expensive for most villagers. Moreover, a new type of person has entered into the life of these changing villages — the “thug” or “local undesirable,” generally engaged in illegal activity, such as distilling illicit brews, peddling dope, and stealing cattle. Twenty-seven persons causing initial offense to our clients were described by the latter in one or more of the following terms: “thug,” “kasippu” (illicit liquor) dealer, cattle thief, or member of a clique of undesirables.

The above considerations lead us to another fascinating aspect of our sorcery data and that is the problem of meaning. In general, people who came to sorcery centers labor under a sense of felt injustice, and what they expect of the deity is that “justice be done.” If the local institutional structure cannot find means for redressing wrong, then one resorts to supernatural agents to correct wrongs and bring about rightness and justice in the world. Many who come to these shrines to solicit supernatural aid have heard from others that these deities will punish the wrongdoer.  Let me quote from Case No. 4 (Sinigama):

The client had come once before over the loss of a cow. His prayers to Devol Deviyo were answered, for the god punished the people whom he had suspected of stealing it. Since then he has had unshakeable faith in the deity more than in the police ….

Some cases can be quite pathetic and moving. I shall quote from Case No. 6 (Sinigama) to illustrate this aspect of the sorcery data.

Albert Siñño looked a very worried and tormented man for his age. He was about sixty years old. He said for the last ten years his wife, who was younger by twenty-two years, had been carrying on with a man forty years old. Recently Albert performed a tovil ceremony called pūna kapok to get rid of the bad times he was having. During this ceremony his wife’s paramour had come after consuming liquor and threatened him, and in the ensuing fight his arm was broken by the man. Afterwards he had gone to the hospital and avoided the complications of going to the police by telling the doctor that he slipped and fell and broke his arm. He had done this purposely because he was not quite certain that his enemy would be punished properly by the police and the courts in a deserving manner and to his entire satisfaction. He had therefore come to Sinigama after his arm was healed to get this man punished the way he deserved through Devol Deviyo who had a reputation for being severe toward evil men and help put a stop to this disturbance in his marital life as a result of his wife’s continued illicit love affair with this man. All that he wanted from the deity at Sinigama was for the god to mete proper justice to his tormentor and to allow him to live a peaceful and happy life with his wife and children.

The deities at these shrines are arbiters of justice who punish evil doers and redress wrongs. Indeed, the cultural view of these deities is that they will not fulfill the request of the client if his is an unjust one. The human need for justice and righteousness cannot be resolved on the local level owing to the collapse of traditional authority. It is therefore resolved on the supernatural level by divine authority figures whom the human agents supplicate at the sorcery shrine.

 Implication of the study

The distinction between premeditated and spontaneous crime is only a heuristically useful one, in order to show the relevance of sorcery for understanding an aspect of comparative criminology. If, for example, one is interested in the social factors which cause crime this distinction may not be useful. The upshot of the present study is that the pattern as manifest in the official crime statistics of Asian and African societies is grossly inadequate for purposes of comparison, analysis, and inference because they reflect a bias towards spontaneous and unplanned attack, and they tend to ignore an important category of behavior, namely, sorcery. If we are correct, then practically all criminological theories, assumptions, and prejudices relating to non-Western peoples have to be drastically revised or reoriented.

One of the theoretical objections that may be raised against the argument presented in this paper is whether the two practices, sorcery and premeditated murder, are indeed truly comparable, because a person is (from our point of view) only symbolically killed in the former whereas in the latter actual death or injury occurs. It is indeed true that in the one case there is a corpse or a hurt human being and in the other there is not (except when the self-fulfilling prophecy operates and an occasional death may result from sorcery). But the existence of the corpse is not relevant for the analysis because we are interested in how the motive for premeditated violence is canalized through sorcery, rather than through planned violent acts. Insofar as the motive can be, and is, expressed through sorcery, it need not be expressed through the overt practice of planned violence against the victim. Thus in societies where sorcery is practiced one would predict a relative absence of premeditated crime in the official records.

In sorcery practice two things are very clear. First, in almost every instance the client goes to the sorcery shrine with the explicit intention of killing or causing harm to someone, which in general he views as just punishment. Second, he obviously believes that the magical techniques he uses are effective, or at least he believes that in some instances they are effective, because otherwise he would not practice them. If the intention is to harm someone, and this intention is put into effect in magical (sorcery) action which is believed to be effective, surely this implies that aggressive and hostile impulses of individuals are being channeled into sorcery. These impulses are characterized by hostility to a known or unknown enemy, but are not those in which the hostile rage is spontaneous, uncontrolled, and demanding immediate rather than delayed action. That sorcery is a mechanism for the canalization of aggressive impulses can hardly be denied; what could be denied is that the drive which would normally be expressed in premeditated murder and violence would now be canalized into sorcery. My answer is again, I submit, logically plausible, although I cannot prove it. A man who commits premeditated murder is thinking rationally, that is, in terms of criteria of instrumental efficiency; and rationality in this sense involves not only the achievement of the goal, but achievement with minimal risks, costs, and other inconveniences. If cultures like Sri Lanka provide magical alternatives for achieving the goal with little risk involved, then sorcery rather than premeditated crime would be the rational choice for many individuals. It is, however, true that some persons may not believe in the efficacy of sorcery; others may believe that relative certainty in actual calculated murder is preferable to sorcery. These persons choose to ignore the risks and complications involved in actual killing and resort to murder and planned physical violence. But many would adopt what, from the cultural point of view, would be the more rational means of achieving the same goal, and it is this kind of decision-making by large numbers of people that accounts for the relative imbalance in the criminal statistics discussed in this paper. On the other hand, I am not suggesting that all these clients would practice murder if these shrines were not available, since I also affirm that the existence of a mechanism like sorcery motivates many people, not otherwise given to expressing their impulses in this manner, to resort to sorcery.

Another legitimate objection may be raised at this point. It can be argued that while the motive might be the same in the two instances, is the satisfaction of the motive or need, or the reduction of the tension the same? This is a complicated matter and I shall spell out my argument in some detail.

  1. It is important to remember that human beings can often anticipate the consequences of their actions, particularly if these actions are calculated. Thus while it is true that premeditated violence may satisfy the aforesaid motive, the individual may feel that it can produce unpleasant consequences. One tension is reduced but the actor may feel that others may be created anew.
  2. While the cultural belief is strong that sorcery is effective, a personal factor may enhance the cultural belief. The individual who is pushed to the wall by unbearable circumstances wants to believe that it is effective. Thus the personal wish adds strength to the cultural belief in the efficacy of the action. It is the strength of this personal wish, reinforced and validated by the cultural belief that impels some persons to continue to seek redress from other shrines when one shrine has failed to produce results.
  3. What is important therefore is that peoples’ motives are channeled away from premeditated crime into (to us) its symbolic counterpart, namely, sorcery. Yet once the individual goes to the sorcery shrine, and indeed even before going, his motive for killing or hurting his enemy receives some form of gratification. Even before going to the sorcery shrine it is likely that an individual will fantasize the dire consequences that will befall his enemy as a result of his planned sorcery. This wish is immediately reinforced at the sorcery shrine, particularly in Sinigama and Munnesvaram. Once the client goes to Munnesvaram he is impressed by the gossip of other clients, shopkeepers in the vicinity, and priests or their assistants, who talk of the power and vindictiveness of the deity over evil-doers. In Sinigama the priest often loudly and openly brags before his clientele about the power of the deity and the people who have recently died, in the most dramatic circumstances, as a result of his sorcery. Once inside the shrine the client practices cathartic techniques, both in Munnesvaram and in Sinigama, and to a lesser extent in the Muslim shrine of Kahatapitiya. Indeed one is amazed at the sheer sadism and vindictiveness of the curses uttered by the priest, and repeated by the client, against the enemy. It is a kind of action that will reduce the tension in the client and satisfy his immediate desire for vengeance.
  4. For most people steps 2 and 3 are probably all that matters, for after some time complex sociological factors may intervene and resolve on the local level the crisis that instigated the client’s action. For example, the courts may take over the problem and resolve it on that level; arbitrators and mediators may step in and conciliate the persons involved in the conflict; or once the individual has gone to the shrine, returned home, and immersed himself in the routine of village activity, he may give progressively less and less thought to the problem. Time probably is a good healer of social and interpersonal conflicts.
  5. Yet undoubtedly there are others who want concrete results from their magical actions. They may rationalize the failure in one shrine, and seek other and more powerful types of sorcery. For some the self-fulfilling prophecy operates, not simply in the rare dramatic manner as in “voodoo-death,” but in a more subtle and much commoner fashion. In village life, with its poor technological resources, people are constantly beset by personal vicissitudes, such as crop failure, illness, death in the family, or indebtedness. Hence it is possible for a client to interpret any personal misfortune that befalls his enemy, or a member of his family, not as an accident, coincidence, or chance (these terms have little significance for many human beings) but due to the client’s practice of malevolent magic. This is also true of those cases where the client cannot identify his enemy. In such an instance the misfortune that befalls almost any individual he does not like may be interpreted as due to sorcery, and that person himself may be retrospectively identified as the enemy. Thus the effectiveness of magic is constantly validated by the nature of human experience in small communities.

The data from the Kahatapitiya mosque permitted us to test the frequency of successful cases of sorcery. Here the client observes a vow to offer a gift to the saint of the shrine if the sorcery has been successful. This meant that there were two categories of clients — supplicating clients (those who came to practice sorcery) and returning clients (whose sorcery had been successful). At Kahatapitiya during the 60-day period there were 24 returning clients as against 442 supplicating clients. Thus one in every eighteen cases of sorcery was “successful,” which perhaps is sufficient to reinforce the belief in the efficacy of the shrine. However, in 50 per cent (twelve cases) of the successful cases, “successful” was interpreted as the recovery of a lost object or money (ten cases) or an apology or confession by the victim (two cases) without any physical punishment to the victim. Two of the clients interviewed felt dissatisfied with the outcome of sorcery, since the enemy did not suffer physical harm. In twelve of the successful cases the enemy was punished by the deity by death or personal injury. Thus one in 37 cases of sorcery resulted in the true fulfillment of the predicted consequence. We do know, however, that in at least three of these cases the client interpreted some incident that occurred in his village — death or injury to a person — as a result of his sorcery and the victim was retrospectively identified as his enemy. In another case the client voluntarily supplied the information that the prediction was fulfilled in that the enemy broke his leg in a car accident one year after his supplication at the sorcery shrine.

The foregoing argument brings us to another important factor in the behavior of some clients. If a client strongly believes that sorcery is effective, he already imagines the death of his enemy sooner or later. His enemy is alive, but it is just a matter of time before he dies. Thus some individuals at least can indulge in a fantasy satisfaction of the wish and thereby delay the actual satisfaction of seeing the death of the enemy. Nevertheless, it is important to reiterate that the consequent satisfaction of the motive is not crucial to the point of view taken in this paper. What is crucial to the theory is that the intention or motive of the actor has been diverted into the practice of sorcery, rather than into the actual practice of planned physical violence.

Conclusion

What we have said of Sri Lanka is applicable to other parts of Asia and Africa for which we have some evidence.  In a symposium on African homicide and suicide, several anthropologists noted the spontaneous nature of criminal violence.[17]  L. A. and M. C. Fallers have this to say about the Busoga of Southeastern Uganda: “. . . in most of the cases in our sample the deed could not have been planned in advance, but rather was committed on a sudden impulse.”[18] And A. W. Southall says that among the Alur of Northwest Uganda “homicide never seems to be premeditated.”[19]  Jean La Fontaine is even more specific for the Gisu of the eastern border of Uganda: “It is noticeable that in Gisu society killing is rarely the result of a premeditated act… Of the 99 cases studied here there are only four which can be said to be premeditated; this is less than five per cent of the total.”[20] The view of human nature that emerges from these studies seems to me biased and completely implausible. It is highly probable, and the ethnography indicates this, that in these societies also the motive for premeditated murder and violence is being canalized in terms of sorcery and other forms of supernatural action. To get a correct view of the problem anthropologists must move away from the study of sorcery accusations to that of sorcery practice. This is admittedly difficult, but it is an important and rewarding task.

******

Comments are closed.